Do Birds Have Language?
In the cheeps,
trills and tweets of birdsong, scientists find some parallels with human speech
Betsy Mason, Smithsonian
A great tit
sitting on a post in Suffolk, England, calls out. Avalon / Universal
Images Group via Getty Images
In our quest to
find what makes humans unique, we often compare ourselves with our closest
relatives: the great apes. But when it comes to understanding the
quintessentially human capacity for language, scientists are finding that the
most tantalizing clues lay farther afield.
Human language is
made possible by an impressive aptitude for vocal learning. Infants hear sounds
and words, form memories of them, and later try to produce those sounds,
improving as they grow up. Most animals cannot learn to imitate sounds at all.
Though nonhuman primates can learn how to use innate vocalizations in new ways,
they don’t show a similar ability to learn new calls. Interestingly, a small
number of more distant mammal species, including dolphins and bats, do have
this capacity. But among the scattering of nonhuman vocal learners across the
branches of the bush of life, the most impressive are birds — hands (wings?)
down.
Parrots,
songbirds and hummingbirds all learn new vocalizations. The calls and songs of
some species in these groups appear to have even more in common with human
language, such as conveying information intentionally and using simple forms of
some of the elements of human language such as phonology, semantics and syntax.
And the similarities run deeper, including analogous brain structures that are
not shared by species without vocal learning.
These parallels
have motivated an explosion of research in recent decades, says ethologist
Julia Hyland Bruno of Columbia University, who studies social aspects of song
learning in zebra finches. “Lots of people have made analogies between language
and birdsong,” she says.
Hyland Bruno
studies zebra finches because they are more social than most migratory birds —
they like to travel in small bands that occasionally gather into larger groups.
“I’m interested in how it is that they learn their culturally transmitted
vocalizations in these groups,” says Hyland Bruno, coauthor of a paper in the
2021 Annual Review of Linguistics comparing birdsong learning and
culture with human language.
Both birdsong and
language are passed culturally to later generations through vocal learning.
Geographically distant populations of the same bird species can make small
tweaks to their songs over time, eventually resulting in a new dialect — a
process similar in some ways to how humans develop different accents, dialects
and languages.
With all these
similarities in mind, it’s reasonable to ask if birds themselves have language.
It may come down to how you define it.
“I wouldn’t say
they have language in the way linguistic experts define it,” says
neuroscientist Erich Jarvis of the Rockefeller University in New York City, and
a coauthor of Hyland Bruno’s paper on birdsong and language. But for scientists
like Jarvis who study the neurobiology of vocal communication in birds, “I
would say they have a remnant or a rudimentary form of what we might call
spoken language.
“It’s like the
word ‘love.’ You ask lots of people what does it mean, and you’re going to get
a lot of different meanings. Which means that it’s partly a mystery.”
There
are multiple components to spoken language, Jarvis says, and some are
shared by more species than others. A fairly common component is auditory
learning, like a dog figuring out how to respond to the spoken command “sit.”
The vocal learning that humans and some birds do is one of the most specialized
components, but all of them are shared to some degree by other animals, he
says.
The grammar of
bird calls
One key element
of human language is semantics, the connection of words with meanings.
Scientists had long thought that unlike our words, animal vocalizations were
involuntary, reflecting the emotional state of the animal without conveying any
other information. But over the last four decades, numerous studies have shown
that various animals have distinct calls with specific meanings.
Many bird species
use different alarm calls for different predators. Japanese tits, which
nest in tree cavities, have one call that causes their chicks to crouch down to
avoid being pulled out of the nest by crows, and another call for tree snakes
that sends the chicks jumping out of the nest entirely. Siberian
jays vary their calls depending on whether a predatory hawk is seen perching,
looking for prey or actively attacking — and each call elicits a different
response from other nearby jays. And black-capped chickadees change the number
of “dees” in their characteristic call to indicate the relative size and
threat of predators.
Two recent studies
suggest that the order of some birds’ vocalizations may impact their meaning.
Though the idea is still controversial, this could represent a rudimentary form
of the rules governing the order and combination of words and elements in human
language known as syntax, as illustrated by the classic “dog bites man” vs.
“man bites dog” example.
In addition to
alert calls, many bird species use recruitment calls that summon other members
of their species. Both Japanese tits and southern pied
babblers appear to combine alert calls with recruitment calls to create a
sort of call to arms, gathering their compatriots into a mob to harass and
chase off a predator. When the birds hear this call, they approach the caller
while scanning for danger.
Scientists led by
ethologist Toshitaka Suzuki of Kyoto University discovered that the order of
the combined calls matters to the Japanese tits. When Suzuki’s team played a
recorded “alert+recruitment” combo to wild tits, it elicited a much stronger
mobbing response than an artificially reversed “recruitment+alert” call. This
could simply be explained by the birds responding to the combined
alert+recruitment call as its own signal without recognizing the parts of the
combination, but the scientists came up with a clever way to test this
question.
Willow tits have
their own distinct recruitment calls, which Japanese tits also understand and
respond to in the wild. When Suzuki’s team combined the willow tit recruitment
call with the Japanese tit alert call, the Japanese tits responded with the
same combined scanning and approaching behavior — but only if the calls were in
the correct alert+recruitment order.
“These results
demonstrate a new parallel between animal communication systems and human
language,” Suzuki and colleagues wrote in Current
Biology in 2017.
But it’s a matter
of interpretation whether the call combinations of the tits and babblers is
really relevant to discussions of human language, which involves more complex
sequences, says behavioral neuroscientist Adam Fishbein of the University of
California, San Diego.
“If they were
doing something more like language, you would get a whole bunch of different
combinations of things,” Fishbein says. “It’s such a restricted system within
the birds.”
Sounding it out
Fishbein’s own
research with zebra finch song suggests that syntax may not be as important to
birds as it is to humans. “I feel like people have been trying to impose this
human way of thinking about communication on what the birds are doing,” he
says.
Birdsong can be
very complex and tends to have typical sequences and patterns of notes,
syllables and motifs. So birds’ singing may be a closer analog to human
language than the tits’ alert and recruitment calls. To the human ear, parts of
birdsong are reminiscent of word syllables, so it’s easy to assume the order of
those parts is important to the message. But, perhaps surprisingly, we know
very little about how birdsong sequences are perceived by the avian ear.
Fishbein’s research suggests that what birds hear when they listen to birdsong
may be very different from what humans hear.
For his graduate
work at the University of Maryland, Fishbein studied zebra finches that had
been trained to press a button when they heard a change in sounds played to
them. When the birds correctly identified a change, pressing the button got
them a food reward. If they guessed wrong, the lights in their enclosure went
off briefly. Fishbein tested what differences the birds are actually able to
decipher, helping scientists understand what aspects of birdsong are
important to the birds.
In one test,
Fishbein and his colleagues played the finches’ standard song over and over at
regular intervals before slipping in a version of the song with artificially
reordered syllables. This change is easy for humans to hear, but the birds were
surprisingly bad at identifying the shuffled sequence.
The birds
performed much better at another test Fishbein gave them. Within each song
syllable, there are higher-frequency details called “temporal fine structure”
that may be something like what humans perceive as timbre or tone quality.
When the scientists messed with the song’s fine structure by playing one of the
syllables backwards, the finches were “exceedingly” good at catching it.
“It’s a dimension
of sound that they’re much better at hearing than we are,” Fishbein says. “So
they may be tapped into this level of the sound that we’re not tapping into
when we just casually listen to birdsong.”
Our understanding
of what birds hear and what matters to them is limited by what we hear, and as
with a lot of scientific research, the statistical analyses used — in this case
to parse birdsong, says linguist Juan Uriagereka, who worked with Fishbein at
the University of Maryland. “Ten years ago, we didn’t even know what the units
that they were combining were,” he says. “And of course, what we think are the
units, it’s our guess, right?”
Though male zebra
finches all learn the same single song, scientists have found that there is
variation in temporal fine structure among renditions of the standard song,
hinting that the birds have a much richer communication system than we
suspected. “It could be that most of the meaning is packed into the individual
elements,” Fishbein says, “and how they’re arranged may not matter as much for
conveying meaning.”
Mean what you say
Even if some
birds share rudimentary aspects of human language, we still know very little
about what’s actually going on in their minds. Most animal communication
research has focused on describing signals and behavior, which on the surface
can look a lot like human behavior. Determining if the underlying cognitive
processes driving the behavior are also similar is much more challenging.
At the heart of
this question is intentionality. Are animals merely reacting to their
environment, or do they intend to convey information to one another? For
example, upon discovering food a bird may make a characteristic call that
attracts other birds to the food. Was the call the equivalent of “Yay! Food!” —
unintentionally attracting other birds? Or, was it more like, “Hey guys, come
check out the food I found!”?
Signs of
intentionality have been shown in many animals. Ground squirrels, Siamese
fighting fish, chickens and even fruit flies change their signals depending on
who is around to receive them, an indication that they have some voluntary
control over those signals. Other animals seem to intentionally “show” others
something, like a dog who looks back and forth between a human and a bag of
treats or a hidden toy, perhaps even adding a bark to get the human’s attention
first. Ravens also appear to show objects to other ravens by holding them in
their beak — usually only if the other bird is paying attention.
Some of the best
recent evidence for intentional communication in birds comes from
observations of wild Arabian babblers at the Shezaf Nature Reserve in Israel. A
team led by ethologist Yitzchak Ben-Mocha, recorded adult babblers coaxing
fledglings to move to a new shelter. Adults call and wave their wings in front
of fledglings and then move toward the shelter. If a youngster doesn’t follow
immediately or stops along the way, the adult comes back and does the song and
dance again and again until the fledgling complies.
Scientists call
such signals first-order intentional communication. Some researchers argue that
a more relevant precursor to language like ours is second-order intentional
communication. This involves the signaler knowing something about the
receiver’s mind, such as the bird who found food knowing another bird was
unaware of the food and calling to intentionally inform the ignorant bird. As
you may have guessed, this sort of mental attribution is a hard behavior to
test.
Other scientists
are taking a different tack to try to understand what underlies such
communication by comparing the brain structures that enable vocal learning in
songbirds and humans.
Deeper
connections
Despite humans
and birds being only very distantly related — their last common ancestor lived
more than 300 million years ago — they have remarkably similar brain circuitry
for vocal learning. Nonhuman primates, our closest relatives, lack a
specialized circuit for imitating sounds, leading scientists to conclude that
this ability did not come from a common ancestor. It must have evolved
independently in birds — an example of what is known as evolutionary
convergence.
“There is this
assumption that species more closely related to us are going to be most like
us. And that is true for many traits,” says the Rockefeller’s Jarvis. “But it’s
not true for every trait.”
Jarvis studies
the evolution of language by looking at the brains of songbirds. Animals that
make only innate sounds control the musculature that creates those sounds
through a circuit in the brain stem, an area near the spinal cord that
regulates automatic functions like breathing and heartbeat. “What has happened
is humans and songbirds have evolved this new forebrain circuit for learned
sounds that has taken control of the brain stem circuit for innate sounds,”
Jarvis says.
His theory for
how similar vocal-learning circuits evolved multiple times in distant species
is that they were built from an adjacent circuit that controls the
learning of some movements. “The spoken-language brain circuit in humans and the
song-learning circuit in birds,” Jarvis argues, “evolved by a whole duplication
of the surrounding motor pathway.”
How an entire
brain circuit could be duplicated is unclear, he says, but it could be similar
to how genes sometimes are duplicated and then co-opted for other purposes.
However they evolved, vocal-learning birds and humans have these rare analogous
brain circuits that enable them to learn and imitate sound. This suggests that
behavioral scientists who’ve been trying to learn about human language by
studying how distantly related birds such as zebra finches communicate are onto
something.
“I think we
humans tend to overestimate how different we are,” Jarvis says. Even he has
observed zebra finches singing in the lab or a starling singing in a tree and
thought that it just seemed so different from what humans do. “And then a year
later, we’re making a discovery about the connectivity of the circuit, or the
mechanism of how it’s producing the sounds, and it’s so much like humans.”
No comments:
Post a Comment